Publications: SRL: Opinion |
I recently saw an old friend for the first time in many years. We had been Ph.D. students at the same time, both studying science, though in different areas. She later dropped out of graduate school, went to Harvard Law School, and is now a senior lawyer for a major environmental organization. At some point, the conversation turned to why she left grad school. To my utter astonishment, she said it was because it made her feel stupid. After a couple of years of feeling stupid every day, she was ready to do something else.
I had thought of her as one of the brightest people I knew, and her subsequent career supports that view. What she said bothered me. I kept thinking about it and sometime the next day, it hit me. Science makes me feel stupid, too. It’s just that I’ve gotten used to it. So used to it, in fact, that I actively seek out new opportunities to feel stupid. I wouldn’t know what to do without it. I even think it’s supposed to be this way.
Let me explain.
I don’t think students are made to understand how hard it is to do research, and how very, very hard it is to do important research. It’s a lot harder than taking even very demanding courses.
For almost all of us, one of the reasons that we liked science in high school and college is that we were good at it. That can’t be the only reason; fascination with understanding the physical world and an emotional need to discover new things has to enter into it, too. But high school and college science means taking courses, and doing well in courses means getting the right answers on tests. The framework is one in which there are right answers and, if you know those answers, you do well and get to feel smart.
A Ph.D., where you have to do a research project, is a whole different thing. For me, it was a daunting task. How could I possibly frame the questions that would lead to significant discoveries; design and interpret an experiment so that the conclusions were absolutely convincing; foresee difficulties and see ways around them, or, failing that, solve them when they occurred? My Ph.D. project was somewhat interdisciplinary and, for a while, whenever I ran into a problem, I used to pester the various faculty in my department who were experts in the various disciplines that I needed. I remember the day when Henry Taube (who won the Nobel Prize two years later) told me he didn’t know how to solve the problem I was having in his area. I was a third-year grad student and I figured that Taube knew about a thousand times more than I did (conservative estimate). If he didn’t have the answer, nobody did.
That’s when it hit me: nobody did. That’s why it was a research problem. And because it was my research problem, it was up to me to solve it. Once I faced that fact, I solved the problem in a couple of days. (It wasn’t really very hard, I just had to try a few things.) The critical lesson was that the scope of things I didn’t know wasn’t merely vast—it was for all practical purposes infinite. That realization, instead of being discouraging, was liberating. If our ignorance is infinite, the only possible course of action is to muddle through as best we can.
The more comfortable we become with being stupid, the deeper we will wade into the unknown and the more likely we are to make big discoveries.
I’d like to suggest that our Ph.D. programs often do students a disservice in two ways. First, I don’t think students are made to understand how hard it is to do research, and how very, very hard it is to do important research. It’s a lot harder than taking even very demanding courses. What makes it difficult is that research is immersion in the unknown. We just don’t know what we’re doing. We can’t be sure if we’re asking the right question or doing the right experiment until we get the answer or the result. Admittedly, science is made harder by competition for grants and space in top journals. But apart from all of that, doing significant research is intrinsically hard, and changing departmental, institutional, or national policies will not succeed in lessening its intrinsic difficulty.
The second point is that we don’t do a good enough job of teaching our students how to be productively stupid. That if we don’t feel stupid it means we’re not really trying. I’m not talking about “relative stupidity,” where the other students in the class actually read the material, think about it, and ace the exam while you don’t. I’m also not talking about bright people who might be working in areas that don’t match their talents. Science involves confronting our “absolute stupidity.” That kind of stupidity is an existential fact, inherent in our efforts to push our way into the unknown. Preliminary and thesis exams have the right idea when the faculty committee pushes until the student starts getting the answers wrong or gives up and says “I don’t know.” The point of the exam isn’t to see if students get all the answers right. If they do, it’s the faculty who failed. The point is to identify a student’s weaknesses—partly to see where he or she needs to invest some effort and partly to see whether the student’s knowledge fails at a sufficiently high level that he or she is ready to take on a research project.
Productive stupidity means being ignorant by choice. Focusing on important questions puts us in the awkward position of being ignorant. One of the beautiful things about science is that it allows us to bumble along, getting it wrong time after time, and yet feeling perfectly fine as long as we learn something each time. No doubt, this can be difficult for students accustomed to getting the answers right. No doubt, reasonable levels of confidence and emotional resilience help. But I think scientific education might do more to ease what is a very big transition: from learning what other people once discovered to making your own discoveries. The more comfortable we become with being stupid, the deeper we will wade into the unknown and the more likely we are to make big discoveries.
srled [at] seismosoc [dot] org
>.
[Back]
Posted: 20 December 2010